Monday, November 25, 2024

conspiracy resource

Conspiracy News & Views from all angles, up-to-the-minute and uncensored

Conspiracy

Foolproof: A psychological vaccine against fake news

A 2016 poll by the Washington-based research centre Pew revealed that over 65% of Americans report that fake news leaves them feeling confused about basic facts, and 83% of Europeans think fake news is a major problem – with at least half reporting that they encounter misinformation on a weekly basis.

Even conspiracy theories are no longer reserved for the ‘lunatic fringe’: over half of Americans now endorse at least one conspiracy theory. These findings raise bigger questions about our ability to navigate the media ecosystem. How do people come to believe in misinformation? How and why does it spread? And what can we do about it?

I have studied the psychology of persuasion and influence my entire career.

In fact, I have spent a good chunk of my personal life trying to understand the process by which people, including myself, become convinced of the veracity of all kinds of information, especially misinformation.

Most of my family on my mother’s side were executed by the Nazis in the Netherlands. My grandparents were able to escape – just barely – but one of their two sons, my uncle, a young boy, was shot on purpose by a Nazi soldier during a razzia (round-up). He was taken to a nearby hospital. The Dutch resistance managed to sneak him out and a lovely family took him in. My uncle never recovered psychologically from what had happened to him as a child.

After learning about the impact of Nazism on my family, I became fascinated, from a young age, by the process through which people are persuaded of dangerous and harmful propaganda.

I started studying persuasion not to persuade other people but to reverse-engineer the process. What if a better and deeper understanding of the persuasion process could empower people to withstand malicious attempts to influence their opinion? I decided to explore the building blocks of disinformation campaigns to see if I could synthesize a ‘vaccine’.

I am not here to tell you what to believe. Andy Norman, a philosopher at Carnegie Mellon University, refers to me as a “cognitive immunologist“. I quite like this description of my field of research. Just as antigens produce an immune response in the body, psychological antigens can help build resistance to fake news.

The lawyer defending the Capitol rioter Anthony Antonio was not wrong when he suggested that people can catch misinformation much like a disease. On this subject, I have three key propositions:

The first is that there are viruses of the mind and not just the biological kind.

A biological virus is a parasite that attaches itself to the surface of a host cell. It will then inject its own genetic material into the host, hijacking the host cell’s machinery with the goal of reproducing itself.

In a very similar way, misinformation, conspiracy theories, and other dangerous ideas, latch on to the brain and insert themselves deep into our consciousness; they infiltrate our thoughts, feelings, and even our memories. Misinformation can fundamentally alter the way we act and think about the world. The misinformation virus hijacks parts of our basic cognitive machinery.

Unfortunately, just as viruses are notoriously hard to treat (antibiotics don’t work), decades of research show that once misinformation takes root in our brains, it too is extremely difficult to correct. The analogy runs deep.

The second proposition is that, just like regular viral pathogens, fake news must spread from one person to another.

In fact, in 2020 the World Health Organization declared the outbreak of an “infodemic“. By themselves, viruses cannot survive for very long outside of the body. They need a host. The same is true of misinformation.

On its own a misleading tweet or fake news headline doesn’t do much – it needs a susceptible host in order to reproduce and spread. Biological viruses usually spread via contact or respiratory droplets. But viruses of the mind are even more contagious because they can be shared from one individual to the next without the need for any physical contact.

Like regular viruses, they can have dangerous health-threatening consequences, leading to injury and even death. Moreover, the spread of fake news doesn’t only threaten the wellbeing of individuals. It poses serious threats to the integrity of elections and democracies worldwide.

The third and final proposition is that we need an effective antiviral treatment – a psychological vaccine against fake news.

Together with my colleagues at Cambridge University, I have been working on this issue for many years now. Luckily, this vaccine does not require any needles, just an open mind, and is completely free of charge. The theory follows the medical analogy closely.

In the same way that vaccines train the immune system to recognize and fight off foreign invaders, often by introducing weakened or dead strains of a virus, we can inoculate the human mind too. By injecting people with a severely weakened dose of fake news (the virus) and refuting it in advance, over time people can develop mental antibodies – psychological immunity – against misinformation.

Creating a vaccine requires understanding the structure of a virus, the chemicals that encode its genetic information (such as DNA and RNA). Accordingly, my research group has tried to uncover the key techniques that underpin the production of nearly all fake news, establishing a taxonomy of what we call the ‘Six Degrees of Manipulation’: polarizing groups in society; appealing to emotions to manipulate people; floating conspiracy theories; impersonating fake experts and official organisations; and trolling people and online conversations.

You can think of these six techniques as the core building blocks of the fake news virus. Our theory was that you could potentially inoculate people against these underlying techniques, and after many lab experiments, we finally achieved a breakthrough.

But we needed a vehicle to deliver the vaccine – a way to translate the theory into a virtual needle. Through a unique collaboration with game developers, we built a simulation engine. We called it Bad News.

The first of its kind, Bad News is a fully interactive fake news game that mimics a social media environment. The game broke new ground but its logic remains slightly controversial: in order to confer immunity, people needed to be ‘injected’ with a microdose of the fake news virus.

In the game, players are exposed to weakened doses of the key techniques that are used in the spread of misinformation. For a brief moment, the player is in charge of their own fake news empire. In order to test the ‘vaccine’, our research group enrolled tens of thousands of people from all walks of life, from all around the world.

Before they entered the simulation, individuals were asked to evaluate the reliability of a series of fake news headlines.

Here’s an example: The Bitcoin exchange rate is being manipulated by a small group of rich bankers. #InvestigateNow.

This fake Twitter post uses the ‘conspiracy’ technique: it casts doubt on the mainstream narrative by blaming a cabal of elites who are secretly plotting to accomplish a sinister goal.

After participating in our intervention (the vaccine), people are shown another series of fake headlines, including ones they’ve never seen before (the invaders). This time around they are less easily fooled.

Psychological antibodies have started forming, which allow participants to better resist fake news because they understand the underlying tactics. The theory of psychological inoculation has turned the world of fact-checkers upside down.

Unlike fact-checking (or ‘debunking’) the idea here is not to issue corrections after the damage has been done. Like vaccination, when it comes to misinformation there is real value in the notion that an ounce of prevention is worth a pound of cure. We need to play ‘offense rather than defence’. Bad News represents more than just a fun game: it marks the beginning of the new science of ‘prebunking’.

Foolproof: Why We Fall for Misinformation and How to Build Immunity’ is published by 4th Estate/HarperCollins in the UK on the 16 February and by WW Norton in the US on 23 March.

***
This article has been archived for your research. The original version from University of Cambridge news can be found here.